Advertisement
Research Article| Volume 37, ISSUE 3, P426-432, May 2023

Download started.

Ok

Intrinsic Laryngeal Muscle Activity During Subvocalization

Published:February 18, 2021DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2021.01.021

      Summary

      Purpose

      Subvocalization, the low-grade activity of speech articulator muscles while thinking or reading, may mediate phonological representations of verbal material. However, no literature exists that directly measures whether intrinsic laryngeal muscles (ILMs) are active during subvocalization. The possibility of ILM activation during subvocalization has implications for establishing appropriate baselines when experimental conditions involve linguistic features.

      Method

      In two separate studies, forty-five cisgender women completed one or two silentsil tasks (two in the first study, Experiments 1a and 1b, and one in the second, Experiment 2). Fine wire electromyography was used to directly measure ILM activity during an at-rest baseline and silent tasks used to determine whether subvocalization occurred (referred to hereafter as “subvocalization tasks”). Other muscles were measured via surface electromyography: submental muscle in Experiments 1a and 1b, anterior tibialis in Experiment 2, and upper trapezius in all experiments.

      Results

      Interrupted time-series analysis was used to directly measure changes in ILM activity from baseline to the subvocalization tasks. A paired two tailed t-test was used to measure mean differences in ILM activity across conditions for each participant. Some individuals displayed statistically significant increases from baseline during subvocalization tasks, whereas others displayed decreases. Cohen's d was used to calculate the effect size for each muscle across the three subvocalization conditions. Of the 21 muscles measured across three experiments, five yielded a small mean effect size, and the effect sizes for the remaining 16 muscles were negligible. At a group level, only the right cricothyroid showed statistically significant changes (Experiment 1b).

      Conclusion

      The ILM responses during subvocalization vary in both magnitude and direction. Most but not all changes can be described as negligible. For future studies of ILM activity during conditions that involve linguistic processing, investigators should consider the idiosyncratic variation during subvocalization when determining the most appropriate baseline task.

      Key Words

      To read this article in full you will need to make a payment

      Purchase one-time access:

      Academic & Personal: 24 hour online accessCorporate R&D Professionals: 24 hour online access
      One-time access price info
      • For academic or personal research use, select 'Academic and Personal'
      • For corporate R&D use, select 'Corporate R&D Professionals'

      Subscribe:

      Subscribe to Journal of Voice
      Already a print subscriber? Claim online access
      Already an online subscriber? Sign in
      Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect

      REFERENCES

        • McGuigan FJ.
        Covert oral behavior during the silent performance of language tasks.
        Psychol Bulletin. 1970; 74: 309-326
        • Aarons L.
        Subvocalization aural EMG feedback in reading.
        Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1971; 33: 271-306https://doi.org/10.2466/pms
        • Baddeley A
        • Eldridge M
        • Lewis V.
        The role of subvocalisation in reading.
        The Quarterly J Experimental Psychol. 1981; 33A: 439-454
        • Laffey J
        • Kelly D.
        Multi-discipline analysis of subvocalization effects on comprehension.
        in: McNinch G. American Reading Forum Online Yearbook. Am. Read. Forum Inc, Sarasota, FL. Vol. II. 1982: 105-109
        • Slowiaczek ML
        • Clifton C.
        Subvocalization and reading for meaning.
        J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav. 1980; 15: 573-582https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90628-3
        • Daneman M
        • Newson M.
        Assessing the importance of subvocalization during normal silent reading.
        Reading and Writing. 1992; 4: 55-77
        • Bosshardt H-G.
        Subvocalization and reading rate differences between stuttering and nonstuttering children and adults.
        J Speech, Language, and Hearing Res. 1990; 33: 776-785https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3304.776
        • Martin M.
        Speech recoding in silent reading.
        Memory & Cognition. 1978; 6: 108-114https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197435
        • Pich J.
        The role of subvocalization in rehearsal and maintenance of rhythmic patterns.
        The Spanish J Psychol. 2000; 3: 63-67https://doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600005552
        • Smith JD
        • Wilson M
        • Reisberg D.
        The role of Subvocalization in Auditory Imagery. Pergamon.
        Neuropsychologia. 1995; 33: 1433-1454
        • Brodsky W
        • Kessler Y
        • Rubinstein BS
        • et al.
        The mental representation of music notation: Notational Audiation.
        J Experimental Psychol: Human Perception and Performance. 2008; 34: 427-445https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.427
        • Bruder C
        • Wöllner C.
        Subvocalization in singers: Laryngoscopy and surface EMG effects when imagining and listening to song and text.
        Psychol of Music. 2019; (Published online): 1-14https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735619883681
        • Edfeldt ÅW.
        Silent Speech and Silent Reading.
        University of Chicago Press. Chicago, IL, 1960 (Werner Å, ed.)
        • Hardyck CD
        • Petrinovich LF.
        Subvocal speech and comprehension level as a function of the difficulty level of reading material I.
        J Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1970; 9: 647-652
        • Helou LB
        • Rosen CA
        • Wang W
        • et al.
        Intrinsic laryngeal muscle response to a public speech preparation stressor.
        J Speech, Language, and Hearing Res. 2018; 61: 1525-1543https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_jslhr-s-17-0153
        • Helou LB
        • Wang W
        • Ashmore RC
        • et al.
        Intrinsic laryngeal muscle activity in response to autonomic nervous system activation.
        Laryngoscope. 2013; 123: 2756-2765https://doi.org/10.1002/lary.24109
        • Balhara YS
        • Verma R
        • Gupta C.
        Gender differences in stress response: Role of developmental and biological determinants.
        Industrial Psychiatry J. 2012; 20: 4https://doi.org/10.4103/0972-6748.98407
        • Taylor SE
        • Klein LC
        • Lewis BP
        • et al.
        Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight.
        Psychological Rev. 2000; 107: 411-429
        • Girdler SS
        • Turner JR
        • Sherwood A
        • et al.
        Gender differences in blood pressure control during a variety of behavioral stressors.
        Psychosomatic Medicine. 1990; 52: 571-591
        • Liu X
        • Iwanaga K
        • Shimomura Y
        • et al.
        Different types of circulatory responses to mental tasks.
        J Physiol Anthropol. 2007; 26: 355-364https://doi.org/10.2114/jpa2.26.355
        • Kajantie E
        • Phillips DIW.
        The effects of sex and hormonal status on the physiological response to acute psychosocial stress.
        Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006; 31: 151-178https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.002
        • Munin MC
        • Murry T
        • Rosen CA.
        Laryngeal electromyography: diagnostic and prognostic applications.
        Otolaryngol Clin N Am. 2000; 33: 759-770
        • Gaeta L
        • Brydges CR.
        An examination of effect sizes and statistical power in speech, language, and hearing research.
        J Speech, Language, and Hearing Res. 2020; (Published online April 28): 1-9https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_jslhr-19-00299