Advertisement
Research Article|Articles in Press

The Key to Singing Off-Key: The Trained Singer and Pitch Perception Distortion

  • Sarah R Kervin
    Correspondence
    Address correspondence and reprint requests to Sarah R Kervin, New York University, Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, 665 Broadway #9, New York, NY, 10012
    Affiliations
    New York University, Department of Communicative Sciences and Disorders, 665 Broadway #9, New York, NY, 10012

    Grabscheid Voice and Swallowing Center, New York Eye and Ear Infirmary of Mount Sinai, 380 2nd Ave, 9th Fl, New York, NY, 10010
    Search for articles by this author
Published:January 31, 2023DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2022.12.016

      Summary

      Objectives

      Pitch perception distortion (PPD) is a novel term describing a phenomenon in which an amplified, accompanied singer's perception of their sung pitch relative to band or accompaniment becomes ambiguous, leading to one of two conditions: a) the singer believes they are out of tune with the accompaniment, but are in tune as perceived by a listener, or b) the singer believes they are in tune with the accompaniment, but are not. This pilot study aims to investigate the existence and incidence of PPD among amplified, accompanied performers and identify associated variables.

      Design/Methods

      115 singers were recruited to participate in an online survey, which collected information on musical training, performance environment, and PPD experience.

      Results

      Reported PPD incidence was 68%, with 92% of respondents indicating that PPD occurred rarely. The factors reported as most associated with PPD experiences included loud stage volume, poor song familiarity, singing outside one's habitual pitch range, and singing loudly. Contrary to previous studies and our hypotheses, no association was found between modality of auditory feedback (e.g., in-ears versus floor monitors) and incidence of PPD. Additionally, higher levels of training were found to be associated with higher incidence of PPD.

      Conclusions

      The reported incidence supports that PPD exists beyond chance and anecdotal experience. In light of the highly trained sample, the data suggest that pitch accuracy in accompanied, amplified performance may be more associated with aural environment–specifically loud stage volume–and a highly trained singer's tuning strategy in response to that environment rather than a singer's mastery of vocal intonation skills in isolation. Loud stage volume was implicated as a primary factor associated with PPD, which may be related to the stapedius reflex. Future investigations will target attempted elicitation of PPD in trained singers after establishing baseline auditory reflex thresholds and objective measurements of intonation accuracy.

      Key Words

      To read this article in full you will need to make a payment

      Purchase one-time access:

      Academic & Personal: 24 hour online accessCorporate R&D Professionals: 24 hour online access
      One-time access price info
      • For academic or personal research use, select 'Academic and Personal'
      • For corporate R&D use, select 'Corporate R&D Professionals'

      Subscribe:

      Subscribe to Journal of Voice
      Already a print subscriber? Claim online access
      Already an online subscriber? Sign in
      Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect

      REFERENCES

        • Erickson ML.
        Can inexperienced listeners hear who is flat? The role of timbre and vibrato.
        J Voice. 2016; 30: 638.e9-638.e20https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2015.07.014
        • Estis JM
        • Dean-Claytor A
        • Moore RE
        • et al.
        Pitch-matching accuracy in trained singers and untrained individuals: the impact of musical interference and noise.
        J Voice. Mar 2011; 25: 173-180https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2009.10.010
        • Larrouy-Maestri P
        • Magis D
        • Grabenhorst M
        • et al.
        Layman versus professional musician: who makes the better judge?.
        PLoS One. 2015; 10e0135394https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0135394
        • Bottalico P
        • Graetzer S
        • Hunter EJ.
        Effect of training and level of external auditory feedback on the singing voice: pitch inaccuracy.
        J Voice. 2017; 31: 122.e9-122.e16https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2016.01.012
        • Erdemir A
        • Rieser J.
        Singing without hearing: the use of auditory and motor information when singers, instrumentalists, and nonmusicians sing a familiar tune.
        Music Percept. 2016; 33https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2016.33.5.546
        • Frühholz S
        • Trost W
        • Constantinescu I
        • et al.
        Neural dynamics of Karaoke-like voice Imitation in singing performance.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2020; 14: 135https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00135
        • Hutchins S
        • Larrouy-Maestri P
        • Peretz I.
        Singing ability is rooted in vocal-motor control of pitch.
        Atten Percept Psychophys. 2014; 76: 2522-2530https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0732-1
        • Pfordresher PQ.
        Musical training and the role of auditory feedback during performance.
        Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012; 1252: 171-178https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06408.x
        • Watts C
        • Moore R
        • McCaghren K.
        The relationship between vocal pitch-matching skills and pitch discrimination skills in untrained accurate and inaccurate singers.
        J Voice. 2005; 19: 534-543https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2004.09.001
        • Berkowska M
        • Dalla Bella S.
        Reducing linguistic information enhances singing proficiency in occasional singers.
        Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009; 1169: 108-111https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04774.x
        • Furuya S
        • Ishimaru R
        • Nagata N.
        Factors of choking under pressure in musicians.
        PLoS One. 2021; 16e0244082https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244082
        • Larrouy-Maestri P
        • Morsomme D.
        The effects of stress on singing voice accuracy.
        J Voice. 2014; 28: 52-58https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2013.07.008
        • Federman J
        • Ricketts T.
        Preferred and minimum acceptable listening levels for musicians while using floor and in-ear monitors.
        J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008; 51: 147-159https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/011)
        • Mürbe D
        • Pabst F
        • Hofmann G
        • et al.
        Significance of auditory and kinesthetic feedback to singers' pitch control.
        Journal of voice: official journal of the Voice Foundation. 2002; 16 (04/01): 44-51https://doi.org/10.1016/S0892-1997(02)00071-1
        • Pfordresher PQ.
        Auditory feedback in music performance: the role of transition-based similarity.
        J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2008; 34: 708-725https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.708
        • Ternström S
        • Sundberg J
        • Colldén A.
        Articulatory Fo perturbations and auditory feedback.
        J Speech Hear Res. 1988; 31: 187-192https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3102.187
        • D'Amario S
        • Howard DM
        • Daffern H
        • et al.
        A longitudinal study of intonation in an a cappella singing quintet.
        J Voice. 2020; 34: 159.e13-159.e27https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2018.07.015
        • Graves JE
        • Oxenham AJ.
        Familiar tonal context improves accuracy of pitch interval perception.
        Front Psychol. 2017; 8: 1753https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01753
        • Pfordresher PQ.
        Auditory feedback in music performance: the role of melodic structure and musical skill.
        J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2005; 31: 1331-1345https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1331
        • Warrier CM
        • Zatorre RJ.
        Influence of tonal context and timbral variation on perception of pitch.
        Percept Psychophys. 2002; 64: 198-207https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195786
        • Duvvuru S
        • Erickson M.
        The effect of timbre, pitch, and vibrato on vocal pitch-matching accuracy.
        J Voice. 2016; 30: 378.e1-378.e12https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2015.05.011
        • Vurma A
        • Raju M
        • Kuuda A.
        Does timbre affect pitch?: Estimations by musicians and non-musicians.
        Psychol Music. 2011; 39: 291-306https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735610373602
        • Watts CR
        • Hall MD.
        Timbral influences on vocal pitch-matching accuracy.
        Logoped Phoniatr Vocol. 2008; 33: 74-82https://doi.org/10.1080/14015430802028434
        • Bartlett I.
        Reflections on contemporary commercial singing: an insider's perspective.
        Voice Speech Rev. 2014; 8: 27-35https://doi.org/10.1080/23268263.2013.829711
        • Sayles M
        • Winter IM.
        Reverberation challenges the temporal representation of the pitch of complex sounds.
        Neuron. 2008; 58: 789-801https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.03.029
        • Yadav M
        • Cabrera D.
        Autophonic loudness of singers in simulated room acoustic environments.
        J Voice. 2017; 31: 388.e13-388.e25https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2016.09.016
        • Hutchins SM
        • Peretz I.
        A frog in your throat or in your ear? Searching for the causes of poor singing.
        J Exp Psychol Gen. 2012; 141: 76-97https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025064
        • Crocco L
        • McCabe P
        • Madill C.
        Principles of motor learning in classical singing teaching.
        J Voice. 2020; 34: 567-581https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2018.12.019
        • Verdolini K.
        Principles of skill acquisition applied to voice training.
        Vocal vis: Views voice. 1997; : 65-80
        • Amir O
        • Amir N
        • Kishon-Rabin L.
        The effect of superior auditory skills on vocal accuracy.
        J Acoust Soc Am. 2003; 113: 1102-1108https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1536632
        • Beck SL
        • Rieser J
        • Erdemir A.
        Singing without hearing: a comparative study of children and adults singing a familiar tune.
        Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, Brain. 2017; 27 (12R): 131
        • Granot RY
        • Israel-Kolatt R
        • Gilboa A
        • et al.
        Accuracy of pitch matching significantly improved by live voice model.
        J Voice. 2013; 27: 390.e13-390.e20https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2013.01.001
        • Lévêque Y
        • Giovanni A
        • Schön D.
        Pitch-matching in poor singers: human model advantage.
        J Voice. 2012; 26: 293-298https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2011.04.001
        • Moore RE
        • Estis J
        • Gordon-Hickey S
        • et al.
        Pitch discrimination and pitch matching abilities with vocal and nonvocal stimuli.
        J Voice. 2008; 22: 399-407https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2006.10.013
        • Pfordresher PQ
        • Mantell JT.
        Singing with yourself: evidence for an inverse modeling account of poor-pitch singing.
        Cogn Psychol. 2014; 70: 31-57https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.12.005
        • Békésy GV.
        The structure of the middle ear and the hearing of one's own voice by bone conduction.
        J Acoust Soc Am. 1949; 21: 217-232https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1906501
        • Pörschmann C.
        Influences of bone conduction and air conduction on the sound of one's own voice.
        Acustica. 2000; 86: 1038-1045
        • Reinfeldt S
        • Ostli P
        • Håkansson B
        • et al.
        Hearing one's own voice during phoneme vocalization–transmission by air and bone conduction.
        J Acoust Soc Am. 2010; 128: 751-762https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3458855
        • Reinfeldt S
        • Stenfelt S
        • Håkansson B.
        Estimation of bone conduction skull transmission by hearing thresholds and ear-canal sound pressure.
        Hear Res. 2013; 299: 19-28https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2013.01.023
        • Vurma A.
        The timbre of the voice as perceived by the singer him-/herself.
        Logoped Phoniatr Vocol. 2014; 39: 1-10https://doi.org/10.3109/14015439.2013.775334
        • Smith DJ
        • S C.
        • Guenther F.H.
        • et al.
        Contributions of auditory and somatosensory feedback to vocal motor control.
        J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2020; 63: 2039-2053https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00296
        • Lovetri J.
        Contemporary commercial music.
        J Voice. 2008; 22: 260-262https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2006.11.002
      1. Qualtrics.
        2020
      2. Microsoft Excel.
        2018
      3. IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, Version 27.0.
        IBM Corp, 2020
        • Kim JH
        • Choi I.
        Choosing the Level of significance: a decision-theoretic approach.
        Abacus. 2019;
        • Davies RA.
        Chapter 11 - Audiometry and other hearing tests.
        in: Furman JM Lempert T Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier, 2016: 157-176
      4. Atcherson S, Moller A. Hearing: Anatomy, Physiology, and Disorders of the Auditory System (3rd ed.) Aage R. Møller ISBN 13-978-1-59756-427-4ISBN 10-1-59756-427-3 Plural Publishing, Inc. San Diego, USA. Int J.Audiol. 01/29 2015;54(Chapter 8):1. https://doi.org/10.3109/14992027.2014.996828

        • Liden G
        • Nordlund B
        • Hawkins Jr, JE
        Significance of the stapedius reflex for the understanding of speech.
        Acta Otolaryngol Suppl. 1964; 188 (+): 275https://doi.org/10.3109/00016486409134576
        • Dalla Bella S
        • Berkowska M
        Singing proficiency in the majority: normality and "phenotypes" of poor singing.
        Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009; 1169: 99-107https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04558.x
        • McDermott JH
        • Oxenham AJ.
        Music perception, pitch, and the auditory system.
        Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008; 18: 452-463https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.09.005
        • Pfordresher PQ
        • Brown S
        • Meier KM
        • et al.
        Imprecise singing is widespread.
        J Acoust Soc Am. 2010; 128: 2182-2190https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3478782
        • Dahlhaus C.
        Studies on the Origin of Harmonic Tonality.
        Princeton University Press, 2014
        • Brattico E
        • Tervaniemi M
        • Näätänen R
        • et al.
        Musical scale properties are automatically processed in the human auditory cortex.
        Brain Res. 2006; 1117: 162-174https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.023
        • Fujioka T
        • Trainor LJ
        • Ross B
        • et al.
        Musical training enhances automatic encoding of melodic contour and interval structure.
        J Cogn Neurosci. 2004; 16: 1010-1021https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929041502706
        • Marmel F
        • Perrin F
        • Tillmann B.
        Tonal expectations influence early pitch processing.
        J Cogn Neurosci. 2011; 23: 3095-3104https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2011.21632
        • Wu X
        • Zhang B
        • Wei L
        • et al.
        Event-related potential correlates of auditory feedback control of vocal production in experienced singers.
        Neuroreport. 2020; 31: 325-331https://doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0000000000001410
        • Zarate JM
        • Wood S
        • Zatorre RJ.
        Neural networks involved in voluntary and involuntary vocal pitch regulation in experienced singers.
        Neuropsychologia. 2010; 48: 607-618https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.025
        • Bauer KT.
        The role of kinesthesia in a pedagogy for singing.
        J Sing-Official J National Assoc Teach Sing. 2016; 73: 129-136
        • Lombard E.
        Le signe de l'elevation de la voix.
        Ann Mal de L' Oreille et du Larynx. 1911; XXXVII: 101-119
        • Jones JA
        • Keough D.
        Auditory-motor mapping for pitch control in singers and nonsingers.
        Exp Brain Res. 2008; 190: 279-287https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1473-y
        • Mürbe D
        • Pabst F
        • Hofmann G
        • et al.
        Effects of a professional solo singer education on auditory and kinesthetic feedback–a longitudinal study of singers' pitch control.
        J Voice. 2004; 18: 236-241https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2003.05.001
        • Pfordresher PQ
        • Beasley RT.
        Making and monitoring errors based on altered auditory feedback.
        Front Psychol. 2014; 5: 914https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00914
        • Hutchins S
        • Peretz I.
        Perception and action in singing.
        Prog Brain Res. 2011; 191: 103-118https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-444-53752-2.00010-2
        • Zarate JM
        • Delhommeau K
        • Wood S
        • et al.
        Vocal accuracy and neural plasticity following micromelody-discrimination training.
        PLoS One. 2010; 5: e11181https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0011181
        • Keough D
        • Jones JA.
        The sensitivity of auditory-motor representations to subtle changes in auditory feedback while singing.
        J Acoust Soc Am. 2009; 126: 837-846https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3158600
        • Pfordresher PQ
        • Chow K.
        A cost of musical training? Sensorimotor flexibility in musical sequence learning.
        Psychon Bull Rev. 2019; 26: 967-973https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1535-5
        • Ternström S
        • Sundberg J.
        How loudly should you hear your colleagues and yourself.
        J Acoust Soc Am. 1984; 75 (S9-S9)https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2021696
        • Tonkinson S.
        The Lombard effect in choral singing.
        J Voice. 1994; 8: 24-29https://doi.org/10.1016/S0892-1997(05)80316-9